In October 2019 the World Animal Protection published the report “Behind the smile: the multi-billion dollar dolphin entertainment industry” promoting the end of the dolphin industry. The central argument to request the closing of all the dolphin venues is that the dolphins suffer in dolphinaria, but the report does not give any scientific prove to support this accusation. In failing to support its central accusation the conclusions and requests of the whole report can be considered invalid.
The report uses misleading statements, like for example “Throughout the world cetaceans – dolphins, whales and porpoises – are being taken from the wild or bred in captivity to be used for entertainment in tourism venues”. This statement is misleading especially when talking about European dolphinaria. The majority (over 75%) of the dolphins living in the parks of the European Association for Aquatic Mammals (EAAM) have been born under human care[1]. The remaining animals are founder stock that may have been acquired as long ago as the 1960’s. No EAAM Park has imported a dolphin from the wild since 2003.
EAAM institutions are successfully increasing the dolphin population in human care through breeding and cooperative exchanges. However, the importation of dolphins from the wild is not prohibited. CITES permits the import/export of bottlenose dolphins, including wild dolphins, where the exporting government finds that the export will not be detrimental to the survival of the species in the wild. The European Union imposes stricter measures for all cetaceans, however, importation is permitted for non-commercial purposes including research, education and breeding purposes for which conservation benefits will accrue to the species concerned.
The report also states that “From their traumatic capture from the wild to being bred for confinement in grossly inadequate conditions, dolphins and other cetaceans suffer immensely in captivity” which is absolutely false, as the vast majority of dolphins under human care have been born in dolphinaria, and they haven’t had any experience in the sea. The report does not present any scientific evidence to support this statement.
Other of the arguments in the report says that “Keeping dolphins in captivity for entertainment offers no genuine benefit to conservation and scant educational benefits” The shows with dolphins and orcas introduce educational elements, even though not all contents are for educational purposes, and are carried out to capture the attention of a public that has no or little previous knowledge of animals. The main aim of the shows is to awaken empathy in ways that build a bond between species and visitors.
The report has a specific chapter titled “Dolphin suffering for entertainment”, where one would expect to find all the arguments supporting the idea that dolphins suffer just because they are in captivity. But the fact is that that the chapter is just a collection of speculations without any scientific background.
For example, this chapter states “In the wild, species such as bottlenose dolphins often have home ranges exceeding 100 square km2, though ranges vary greatly. Some populations have average home ranges in excess of 400 square km3”. The authors seem to believe that cetaceans swim hundreds of miles because they have a physiological need to do so, but this is not the case they only do it forced by the need to get food. If they are able to find food in a small, shallow area, they remain in the same place and stop doing deep dives for long periods. This has been proven with killer whales tracked with satellite tags in Gibraltar Strait, where the animals have plenty of food in a small area, and they did not travel more than 10 miles per day[2],[3].
This argument seems to make us believe that cetaceans swim 100 miles a day for fun, and if the only swim 50, they will be half as happy. If a dolphin can find food by swimming 50 miles in one day instead of swimming 100, it is not known whether or not it will be half as happy, but certainly it will have expended half the energy. That energy can be invested into the survival of their offspring.
The report also mentions the maximum diving capacity recorded in a dolphin, and states “When the natural ranges of wild dolphins are compared to the miniscule spaces afforded to them in captivity, it’s clear that it is impossible to provide adequate conditions”. In zoological settings, the depth dimensions of habitats for bottlenose dolphins reflect those of the bays and estuaries in which they typically are found. Dives of bottlenose dolphins typically last from 20 to 40 seconds. The depth of dives depends on the habitat in which the dolphins are found.
Bottlenose dolphins are generally found in bays, in tidal waters, and along open ocean beaches, often at depths of 3 meters or less. While dolphins can dive longer and deeper where motivated by the need to forage or to protect themselves from predators, they do not necessarily need to do so when these factors are absent[4],[5]. Moreover, the depth of pools is only one of the many factors that can influence, but not by itself determine, the well-being of dolphins. Regarding other species, like orcas, when they have the opportunity to feed a few tens of meters, they do not dive deeper for pleasure. Data obtained from markers with depth sensors show that they only make deep dives to catch their prey, and the rest of the dives are shallower than 20 m.[6]
Regarding the water quatily, the report by World Animal Protection suggests “Water treatment methods such as ozonation and chlorination are used in the dolphins’ tanks. These maintain the water clarity needed for visitors to see the animals clearly and neutralize the bacteria from large quantities of animal waste products. The use of harsh chemicals like these can cause an array of health issues, particularly of the eyes and skin”. This statement is again pure speculation unsupported by scientific data. The products that are used to reduce the proliferation of bacteria in the water (like chlorine and ozone), if used properly, have no bearing on the animals. If chlorine is extracted from the seawater itself (using for example eclocid machines as in Loro Parque) the chlorine comes from natural seawater and there is no need to add chemicals to purify water. Wild dolphins are exposed to pathogens that can cause different skin conditions, much more dramatic and painful that any skin lesion found in a dolphinarium. If the skin and eye problems would be so frequent in dolphinariums, after visiting hundreds of them worldwide the report should have pictures to illustrate the argument. But there is no a single picture of health issues found in dolphins under human care, which clearly demonstrates that the whole argument is false.
The report includes a statement about the negative effects of noise pollution in the dolpinariums, arguing “Stress caused by noise pollution is a concern for captive dolphins. Often, their tanks are located near sources of loud noise, such as loudspeakers which blast out music during performances.” This statement is also false. The sound from the music is mainly reflected by the water surface and it has been demonstrated that their impact underwater is negligible[7]. In fact the available scientific evidence also demonstrates that, when properly isolated, the noise from the pumps does not reach the pools. The most recently published scientific research [8] comparing the underwater noise pollution of 14 dolphinariums in USA proves that measurements of noise in cetacean pools show noise levels comparable to that found in the sea under normal conditions (with low human disturbance). The noise is much higher in sea areas where human activity is intense, and in fact alterations in dolphin, orca and beluga vocal behaviour associated with noisy human activities like whale watching, have been described.[9],[10], [11], [12]
The report presents a detailed explanation about the intelligence of the dolphins including details about the use of signature whistles (described and confirmed thanks to the zoo housed dolphins), the potential information transmission using sounds (demonstrated in many other species like for example birds, bats, frogs crickets, etc.), are capable to plan events (also shown in other species like birds) and also are capable of self recognition in the mirror. The authors highlight this question explaining that it occurs even before than in human children, but fail to add that this very same mirror self recognition has been proven also in other species, for example magpies or even ants !!! Hence, the neurologists do not agree that the intelligence of a dolphin can be comparable with a human being just because they pass the mirror test. Other ways, they should admit that magpies or ants are also as clever as three year old children. The goal of this detailed description on the intelligence of the dolphins has a clear motivation, support the argument that “The high level of intelligence demonstrated by bottlenose dolphins and other cetacean species makes their confinement and use for entertainment highly unethical”. But this statement is undermined by the fact that many other species like frogs, crickets, parrots, magpies or ants have passed the very same cognition tests. On the other hand, there is a debate among the welfare experts about the influence of the “intelligence” in the adaptation to zoo settings. Some experts sustain that a high cognitive level could be a positive factor regarding the adaptation to captivity. At this moment there is no scientific evidence to support none of the two hypothesis.
Regarding the stress produced by the behavioral restriction of the cetaceans the report states “Kept in tiny, featureless concrete tanks, dolphins are denied freedom of movement and the ability to carry out natural behaviors.” This affirmation is not totally correct, as the dolphins can express many natural behaviors (like swimming, copulating, giving birth, taking care and feeding calves, echolocating, whistling, etc.). There is a strong consensus in the animal welfare community that the animals do not need to express their complete behavioral repertoire to be in good welfare status. Being attacked by a predator, running away from threats or being killed by another dolphin are natural behaviors that are clearly not necessary to reach a good welfare status. Thus, it is clear that not all the behaviors are related with welfare, but only some essential ones. How this essential behaviors relate with welfare should be supported by proper ethological observations based on the scientific method, not in unfounded speculations.
The report also states “Group sizes in captive facilities usually consist of two to four dolphins per tank only. This is much smaller than average pod sizes in the wild and it likely impacts their social behavior.” This is a very vague statement, specially considering the fact that the authors include a list of facilities visited with the number of dolphins held. Anybody can check that only 28% of the facilities house 4 or less dolphins. That means 72% of the facilities have bigger groups, which clearly contradicts their own statement. If the majority of facilities have groups bigger than 5 animals the social behavior should not be compromised. The authors state in the report “Our research identified 233 dolphin venues keeping 1,770 dolphins”, if the average number of dolphins per venue is calculated the value is 7.6, which contradicts their assertion that group sizes usually consist of two to four dolphins.
Regarding the aggression related stress the report makes a curios statement “Dolphins rake each other in the wild as part of determining social hierarchy; however, in most mammals, once dominance hierarchies are established, they remain relatively stable, reducing repeated aggression.” This is not supported by scientific evidence, on the contrary is well known that dolphins live in a fission-fussion society where hierarchy changes and is constantly challenged. Rake marks in dolphins are fairly common, it has been calculated that over 60% of individuals have them [13] (the remaining 40% are usually young individuals). This is so common that has been used by some researchers to identify specimens or even to evaluate differential aggression by gender[14],[15].
The authors also report that “Injuries from teeth raking in captivity have been fatal at times”, but fail to explain that it only happened once in the 80s and never after. While an open wound can be an entry for pathogens into the bloodstream of cetaceans, this is only dangerous in contaminated waters. The daily hygienic control of the water in dolphinaria (in Spain the water quality in dolphinaria is controlled 50 times more than pool water for human use) makes this risk negligible. The cause of the death of the dolphin was an infection by Clostridium perfringens, the presence of this pathogenic bacteria is regularly monitored since 1978[16].
The report also emphasizes the fact that “the notion of captive dolphin venues being of value to wild dolphin populations, however, is misleading” because bottlenose dolphin are not endangered. And “No zoos or aquaria currently engage in captive breeding programmes designed to increase wild cetacean populations”. Obviously zoos and aquaria do not release bottlenose dolphins because it would be against the regulations of the International Union for Nature Conservation, as it would put in risk the wild populations of dolphins. Nevertheless, the breeding of bottlenose dolphins or even killer whales under human care provides knowledge and expertise that will be necessary to save the critically endangered cetacean species in the next decades. The vaquita, a critically endangered porpoise species, is the best example of how important is this knowledge and expertise. When the last desperate attempt to establish a breeding group to save the species was made, the experts involved were zoo and aquarium staff. Unfortunately it was too late for the vaquita, but the survival of next critically endangered cetaceans will depend mainly in that expertise gained with dolphins.
Regarding the scientific research, the report claims that “dolphin entertainment venues tend to focus on issues which serve to address problems in their industry rather than on conservation or animal welfare”, which is totally false. Just as an example the Annex I includes 324 scientific publications produced from research developed with cetaceans under human care by members of the European Association for Aquatic Mammals.
The authors also mention “the behavioral research, in particular, is questionable with captive subjects due to the constraints put on cetaceans such as small tank sizes and artificial social groupings. These constraints limit their natural behavior and lead to biases in research studies.” Which is absolutely non-sense, the experimental designs are done by researchers from different universities worldwide, and the results are published in peer reviewed journals, which is a prove of scientific quality.
Regarding the comparative health of wild and captive dolphins the authors claim: “when comparing the health of wild and captive dolphins, studies haveshown clear differences. Free-ranging bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Florida appear to have a lower risk of developing metabolic syndrome and insulin resistance when compared to their captive counterparts” which is a clear case of “cherry picking” using only the scientific results that support their pre-conceptions. Hence, the authors fail to include other scientific papers produced by the same research team comparing the very same dolphin groups and that have conclusions that do not support the idea of the better welfare of the wild dolphins. In fact the most recent scientific evidence demonstrates that the immune system of wild dolphins from a coastal population shows clear signs of health stress compared to a group of animals housed on a certified zoo[17]. This is the scientific evidence that the wild dolphins suffer more illness and pathologies than the ones under human care, as it can be expected just by the fact that the dolphinaria have professional veterinary services to take care of the animal health.
The report also has claims about the diet of the cetaceans under human care, which is on the opinion of the authors the cause of the “the health issues of captive dolphins” which often consists of “a limited diet of frozen-thawed fish in a few large meals”. In the opinion of the authors: “Their wild counterparts, however, were found to consume small portions of varied fish species when necessary.” This is an idealized concept of the wild, as the wild dolphins do not consume fish when necessary, but only when they have the chance to capture them. That means in some moments of the day they can consume a lot of fish and maybe starve for several days when conditions are not optimum. Trying to copy this feeding regime does not seem reasonable nor positive for the welfare of the dolphins.
Regarding the quality of the food the authors state “frozen then thawed fish is the main component of captive cetacean diets, but such fish are lower in nutritional value than live fish” which is a clear exaggeration. The nutritional difference of fresh and frozen fish is really subtle, just a small reduction in some vitamins that can be easily supplemented. It is obvious that wild dolphins do not have to receive supplements, but in consuming fresh fish they are exposed to parasites that are absent in frozen fish. The parasite infestation is quite common in wild animals and produces a wide variety of pathologies and enormous suffering. It seems reasonable to save this suffering to the dolphins under human care if the only odd is a vitamin supplement.
Another old fashioned argument that can be found in the report is the dental damage: “Bored and frustrated by their captivity, cetaceans persistently grind their teeth against the concrete of their tanks, or bite down on metal bars between tanks, breaking their teeth. This pattern of repetitive abnormal behavior – a stereotypy – leads to teeth breaking and wearing down to the gums. It is not seen in wild cetaceans.” This statement is totally false. There are many documented cases of dental damage in the wild for different reasons (abrasive food, manipulation of abrasive objects, etc.) there is not a single scientific study that relates dental damage in cetaceans and boredom. In cetaceans teeth typically have extremely limited function in food processing, so how can a broken teeth compromise the welfare of an animal? If the broken tooth does not produce any pain, inflammation or infection, there will not be significative effect in the welfare of the animal. Besides, it is not true that these lesions are not seen in wild cetaceans. The teeth wear is not an exclusive problem of captive killer whales or dolphins, there are many examples of wild killer whales with their tooth worn to the gum[18],[19]. While orca’s teeth in captivity are often damaged, strict veterinary control and daily dental hygiene prevent inflammation, infections and pain. In the case of wild orcas, there is no way to control inflammation and infection, and presumably this produces a painful process in the wild killer whales.
The report is full of contradictions, but when talking about the longevity and mortality rates is when it reaches its maximum levels of dishonesty. The authors recognize that “studies have shown improvements in captive bottlenose dolphin mortality rates over time” and at the same time assert that “dolphins do not live significantly longer than their wild counterparts.” Which is absolutely nonsense, as a lower mortality ration implies a longer life. The most recent scientific evidence makes clear that dolphins have a lower mortality rate and, as a consequence, live longer under human care.[20],[21],[22],[23],[24]
Finally, the authors state “Ultimately, however, the mortality rates of captive cetaceans – whether improved, or comparable to some weakened wild populations – can never be a justification for keeping them in captivity. A long life in a tiny, barren enclosure is not a good life.” Which is clearly the acknowledgement that dolphins do live longer under human care, despite they do not like to recognize it. What the authors do not understand is that the fact that dolphins live longer under human care is not a justification to keep them in dolphinaria. This fact is the final prove that cetaceans do not suffer under human care, because they would not be able to overlive their wild counterparts if the tragic story of a life of stress and suffering would be true. Hence, this is the evidence that the cetaceans do not suffer under human care.
To conclude, within the report it can be found a clear prove that the “researchers” that were inspecting the dolphinaria did not have the skills to evaluate the acgtual situation. The report states that “At Loro Parque in Tenerife, staff who do ’water work’ with dolphins, ie, enter the water with them, carry a cylinder of five minutes’ worth of breathable compressed air to be used in emergencies.”. This statement is blatantly false, the dolphin trainers at Loro Parque have never carried such security equipment. The only staff who carries this safety equipment is the killer whale trainers, not because they do any waterwork, but just in case of an accidental fall in one of the pools. If the observers sent to the parks were unable to distinguish between a dolphin and a killer whale, it is clear that the conclusions of the report are absolutely useless.
–
Annex I
List of scientific publications produced from research with cetaceans under human care
Lima, A., Sébilleau, M., Boye, M., Durand, C., Hausberger, M., & Lemasson, A. (2018). Captive bottlenose dolphins do discriminate human-made sounds both underwater and in the air. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(JAN). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00055
Abramson, J. Z., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., García, L., Colmenares, F., Aboitiz, F., & Call, J. (2018). Imitation of novel conspecific and human speech sounds in the killer whale ( Orcinus orca ). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1871), 20172171. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.2171
Clegg, I. L. K., & Delfour, F. (2018). Can We Assess Marine Mammal Welfare in Captivity and in the Wild? Considering the Example of Bottlenose Dolphins. Aquatic Mammals, 44(2), 181–200. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.44.2.2018.181
Clegg, I. L. K., Rödel, H. G., Cellier, M., Vink, D., Michaud, I., Mercera, B., … Delfour, F. (2017). Schedule of human-controlled periods structures bottlenose dolphin (tursiops truncatus) behavior in their free-time. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 131(3), 214–224. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000059
Harvey, B. S., Dudzinski, K. M., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2017). Associations and the role of affiliative, agonistic, and socio-sexual behaviors among common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Behavioural Processes, 135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.12.013
Clegg, I. L. K., Van Elk, C. E., & Delfour, F. (2017). Applying welfare science to bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Welfare, 26(2), 165–176. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.26.2.165
Abramson, J. Z., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Esteban, J. A., Colmenares, F., Aboitiz, F., & Call, J. (2017). Contextual imitation of intransitive body actions in a Beluga whale (Delphinapterus leucas): A “do as other does” study. PLoS ONE, 12(6). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0178906
Dolphin, B., Clegg, I. L. K., Rödel, H. G., Cellier, M., Vink, D., Michaud, I., … Böye, M. (2017). Schedule of Human-Controlled Periods Structures Bottlenose Dolphin. Journal of Comparative Psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983).
Serres, A., & Delfour, F. (2017). Environmental changes and anthropogenic factors modulate social play in captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Zoo Biology, 36(2), 99–111. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21355
Clegg, I. L. K., Rödel, H. G., & Delfour, F. (2017). Bottlenose dolphins engaging in more social affiliative behaviour judge ambiguous cues more optimistically. Behavioural Brain Research, 322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2017.01.026
Lima, A., Lemasson, A., Boye, M., & Hausberger, M. (2017). Vocal activities reflect the temporal distribution of bottlenose dolphin social and non-social activity in a zoological park. Zoo Biology, 36(6), 351–359. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21387
Levengood, A. L., & Dudzinski, K. M. (2016). Is blood thicker than water? The role of kin and non-kin in non-mother-calf associations of captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Behavioural Processes, 124, 52–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.12.005
Melero, M., Giménez-Lirola, L. G., Rubio-Guerri, C., Crespo-Picazo, J. L., Sierra, E. E., García-Párraga, D., … Sánchez-Vizcaíno, J. M. (2016). Fluorescent microbead-based immunoassay for anti-Erysipelothrix rhusiopathiae antibody detection in cetaceans. Diseases of Aquatic Organisms, 117(3), 237–243. https://doi.org/10.3354/dao02948
Wright, A., Scadeng, M., Stec, D., Dubowitz, R., Ridgway, S., & Leger, J. St. (2016). Neuroanatomy of the killer whale (Orcinus orca): a magnetic resonance imaging investigation of structure with insights on function and evolution. Brain Structure and Function, 1263(May), 1256–1263. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-016-1225-x
Wisniewska, D. M., Teilmann, J., Hermannsen, L., Johnson, M., Miller, L. A., Siebert, U., & Madsen, P. T. (2016). Quantitative measures of anthropogenic noise on harbor porpoises: Testing the reliability of acoustic tag recordings. In Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology (Vol. 875, pp. 1237–1242). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-2981-8_155
Frasier, K. E., Elizabeth Henderson, E., Bassett, H. R., & Roch, M. a. (2016). Automated identification and clustering of subunits within delphinid vocalizations. Marine Mammal Science, 32(3), 911–930. https://doi.org/10.1111/mms.12303
Neto, M. P., Silveira, M., & dos Santos, M. E. (2016). Training bottlenose dolphins to overcome avoidance of environmental enrichment objects in order to stimulate play activities. Zoo Biology, 35(3), 210–215. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21282
Lucke, K., Popper, A. N., Hawkins, A. D., Akamatsu, T., André, M., Branstetter, B. K., … Aran Mooney, T. (2016). Auditory sensitivity in aquatic animals. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139(6), 3097–3101. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4952711
Branstetter, B. K., DeLong, C. M., Dziedzic, B., Black, A., & Bakhtiari, K. (2016). Recognition of Frequency Modulated Whistle-Like Sounds by a Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) and Humans with Transformations in Amplitude, Duration and Frequency. PLOS ONE, 11(2), e0147512. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147512
Smith, H., Frère, C., Kobryn, H., & Bejder, L. (2016). Dolphin sociality, distribution and calving as important behavioural patterns informing management. Animal Conservation. https://doi.org/10.1111/acv.12263
Eskelinen, H. C., Winship, K. A., Jones, B. L., Ames, A. E. M., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2016). Acoustic behavior associated with cooperative task success in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-0978-1
Ravignani, A., Fitch, W. T., Hanke, F. D., Heinrich, T., Hurgitsch, B., Kotz, S. a, … De Boer, B. (2016). What pinnipeds have to say about human speech, music, and the evolution of rhythm. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10(July), 274. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNINS.2016.00274
King, S. L., Guarino, E., Donegan, K., Hecksher, J., & Jaakkola, K. (2016). Further insights into postpartum signature whistle use in bottlenose dolphins ( Tursiops truncatus ). Marine Mammal Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/mms.12317
Izidoro, F. B., & Le Pendu, Y. (2016). Rostrum Contact Behaviors by the Guiana Dolphin (Sotalia guianensis) in Ilhéus, Brazil. Aquatic Mammals, 42(2), 203–209. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.42.2.2016.203
King, S. L., Guarino, E., Keaton, L., Erb, L., & Jaakkola, K. (2016). Maternal signature whistle use aids mother-calf reunions in a bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus. Behavioural Processes, 126, 64–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.03.005
Kremers, D., Célérier, A., Schaal, B., Campagna, S., Trabalon, M., Böye, M., … Lemasson, A. (2016). Sensory Perception in Cetaceans: Part II—Promising Experimental Approaches to Study Chemoreception in Dolphins. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 4(May), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2016.00050
Neto, M. P., Silveira, M., & dos Santos, M. E. (2016). Training bottlenose dolphins to overcome avoidance of environmental enrichment objects in order to stimulate play activities. Zoo Biology, 35(3), 210–215. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21282
García-Párraga, D., Gilabert, J. A., García-Peña, F. J., Álvaro, T., Ros-Rodríguez, J. M., Valls, M., & Encinas, T. (2016). Long-lasting concentrations of cefovecin after subcutaneous and intramuscular administration to Patagonian sea lions (Otaria flavescens). The Veterinary Journal, 208(July 2016), 65–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2015.09.015
King, S. L., Guarino, E., Keaton, L., Erb, L., & Jaakkola, K. (2016). Maternal signature whistle use aids mother-calf reunions in a bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus. Behavioural Processes, 126, 64–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.03.005
Beisner, B. A., Hannibal, D. L., Finn, K. R., Fushing, H., & McCowan, B. (2016). Social power, conflict policing, and the role of subordination signals in rhesus macaque society. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.22945
Lucke, K., Finneran, J. J., Almunia, J., & Houser, D. S. (2016). Variability in Click-Evoked Potentials in Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) and Determination of a Hearing Impairment in a Rehabilitated Killer Whale. Aquatic Mammals, 42(2), 184–192. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.42.2.2016.184
Papale, E., Perez-Gil, M., Castrillon, J., Perez-Gil, E., Ruiz, L., Servidio, A., … Martín, V. (2016). Context specificity of Atlantic spotted dolphin acoustic signals in the Canary Islands. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, (May), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2016.1171256
Kremers, D., Célérier, A., Schaal, B., Campagna, S., Trabalon, M., Böye, M., … Lemasson, A. (2016). Sensory Perception in Cetaceans: Part I—Current Knowledge about Dolphin Senses As a Representative Species. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2016.00049
Kremers, D., Célérier, A., Schaal, B., Campagna, S., Trabalon, M., Böye, M., … Lemasson, A. (2016). Sensory Perception in Cetaceans: Part I—Current Knowledge about Dolphin Senses As a Representative Species. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2016.00049
Pepperberg, I. M., & Nakayama, K. (2016). Robust representation of shape in a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Cognition, 153, 146–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.04.014
Levengood, A. L., & Dudzinski, K. M. (2016). Is blood thicker than water? The role of kin and non-kin in non-mother-calf associations of captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Behavioural Processes, 124, 52–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.12.005
Giménez, J., Ramírez, F., Almunia, J., G. Forero, M., & de Stephanis, R. (2016). From the pool to the sea: Applicable isotope turnover rates and diet to skin discrimination factors for bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 475, 54–61.
Lucke, K., Popper, A. N., Hawkins, A. D., Akamatsu, T., André, M., Branstetter, B. K., … Aran Mooney, T. (2016). Auditory sensitivity in aquatic animals. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139(6), 3097–3101. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4952711
Luís, A. R., Couchinho, M. N., & dos Santos, M. E. (2016). A Quantitative Analysis of Pulsed Signals Emitted by Wild Bottlenose Dolphins. Plos One, 11(7), e0157781. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157781
Hacker, C. E., & Miller, L. J. (2016). Zoo visitor perceptions, attitudes, and conservation intent after viewing African elephants at the San Diego Zoo Safari Park. Zoo Biology, 7(January), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21303
Stavros, K. (2016). Scientific Diving As A Toll in Marine Sciences. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.1.1101.5289
Esteban, R., Verborgh, P., Gauffier, P., Giménez, J., Martín, V., Pérez-Gil, M., … de Stephanis, R. (2016). Using a multi-disciplinary approach to identify a critically endangered killer whale management unit. Ecological Indicators, 66(July), 291–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolind.2016.01.043
Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., Lusseau, D., & McCowan, B. (2016). Parallels of human language in the behavior of bottlenose dolphins. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1605.01661
Lopez Marulanda, J., Adam, O., & Delfour, F. (2016). Modulation of whistle production related to training sessions in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) under human care. Zoo Biology, 35(6). https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21328
Brando, S., Bowles, A. E., Boye, M., Dudzinski, K. M., van Elk, N., Lucke, K., … Wahlberg, M. (2016). Proceedings of marine mammal welfare workshops hosted in the Netherlands and the USA in 2012. Aquatic Mammals, 42(3), 392–416. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.42.3.2016.392
Muraco, H., & Ii, S. a K. (2015). Conceptive Estrus Behavior in Three Bottlenose Dolphins ( Tursiops truncatus ). Animal Behaviror and Cognition, 2(1), 30–48. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.02.03.2015
García-Alvarez, N., Fernández, A., Boada, L. D., Zumbado, M., Zaccaroni, A., Arbelo, M., … Luzardo, O. P. (2015). Mercury and selenium status of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): A study in stranded animals on the Canary Islands. Science of The Total Environment, 536(July 2015), 489–498. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2015.07.040
Clegg, I. L. K., Borger-Turner, J. L., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2015). C-Well: The development of a welfare assessment index for captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Welfare, 24(3), 267–282.
Tomonaga, M., Uwano, Y., Ogura, S., Chin, H., Dozaki, M., & Saito, T. (2015). Which person is my trainer? Spontaneous visual discrimination of human individuals by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). SpringerPlus, 4(1), 352. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40064-015-1147-8
Miedler, S., Fahlman, A., Valls Torres, M., Álvaro Álvarez, T., & Garcia-Parraga, D. (2015). Evaluating cardiac physiology through echocardiography in bottlenose dolphins: using stroke volume and cardiac output to estimate systolic left ventricular function during rest and following exercise. Journal of Experimental Biology, 218(Pt 22), 3604–3610. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.131532
Melorose, J., Perroy, R., & Careas, S. (2015). No Title No Title. Statewide Agricultural Land Use Baseline 2015, 1. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004
Rubio-Guerri, C., García-Párraga, D., Nieto-Pelegrín, E., Melero, M., Álvaro, T., Valls, M., … Sánchez-Vizcaíno, J. M. (2015). Novel adenovirus detected in captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) suffering from self-limiting gastroenteritis. BMC Veterinary Research, 11(1), 367. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-015-0367-z
Ridgway, S., Samuelson Dibble, D., Van Alstyne, K., & Price, D. (2015). On doing two things at once: dolphin brain and nose coordinate sonar clicks, buzzes and emotional squeals with social sounds during fish capture. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 218(Pt 24), 3987–3995. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.130559
Luís, A. R., Couchinho, M. N., & Dos Santos, M. E. (2015). Signature whistles in wild bottlenose dolphins: long-term stability and emission rates. Acta Ethologica, (July 2016), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-015-0230-z
García-Alvarez, N., Fernández, A., Boada, L. D., Zumbado, M., Zaccaroni, A., Arbelo, M., … Luzardo, O. P. (2015). Mercury and selenium status of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): A study in stranded animals on the Canary Islands. Science of The Total Environment, 536, 489–498. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2015.07.040
Melero, M., Rodríguez-Prieto, V., Rubio-García, A., García-Párraga, D., & Sánchez-Vizcaíno, J. M. (2015). Thermal reference points as an index for monitoring body temperature in marine mammals. BMC Research Notes, 8(1), 411. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-015-1383-6
Mishima, Y., Morisaka, T., Itoh, M., Matsuo, I., Sakaguchi, A., & Miyamoto, Y. (2015). Individuality embedded in the isolation calls of captive beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas). Zoological Letters, 1(1), 27. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40851-015-0028-x
Fiorucci, L., Garcia-Parraga, D., Macrelli, R., Grande, F., Flanagan, C., Rueca, F., … Saviano, P. (2015). Determination of the main reference values in ultrasound examination of the gastrointestinal tract in clinically healthy bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Aquatic Mammals, 41(3), 284–294.
Kuczaj, S. A., Frick, E. E., Jones, B. L., Lea, J. S. E., Beecham, D., & Schnöller, F. (2015). Underwater observations of dolphin reactions to a distressed conspecific. Learning & Behavior, 43, 289–300. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0179-9
Melero, M., Crespo-Picazo, J. L., Rubio-Guerri, C., García-Párraga, D., & Sánchez-Vizcaíno, J. M. (2015). First molecular determination of herpesvirus from two mysticete species stranded in the Mediterranean Sea. BMC Veterinary Research, 11(1), 283. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-015-0596-1
Gilles, a, Viquerat, S., Becker, E. a., Forney, K. a., Geelhoed, S. C. V., Haelters, J., … Aarts, G. (2015). Seasonal habitat-based density models for a marine top predator, the harbour porpoise, in a dynamic environment., 7(June 2016), 37. https://doi.org/10.13748/j.cnki.issn1007-7693.2014.04.012
Sveegaard, S., Galatius, A., Dietz, R., Kyhn, L., Koblitz, J. C., Amundin, M., … Teilmann, J. (2015). Defining management units for cetaceans by combining genetics, morphology, acoustics and satellite tracking. Global Ecology and Conservation, 3, 839–850. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gecco.2015.04.002
Kuczaj, S. A., Frick, E. E., Jones, B. L., Lea, J. S. E., Beecham, D., & Schnöller, F. (2015). Underwater observations of dolphin reactions to a distressed conspecific. Learning & Behavior, 43, 289–300. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0179-9
Kuczaj, S. A., Winship, K. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2014). Can bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) cooperate when solving a novel task? Animal Cognition, 18(2). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0822-4
García-Álvarez, N., Boada, L. D., Fernández, A., Zumbado, M., Arbelo, M., Sierra, E., … Luzardo, O. P. (2014). Assessment of the levels of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and organochlorine contaminants in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) from the Eastern Atlantic Ocean. Marine Environmental Research, 100(July), 48–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.marenvres.2014.03.010
García-Álvarez, N., Boada, L., Fernández, A., Zumbado, M., Arbelo, M., Sierra, E., … Luzardo, O. (2014). Assessment of the levels of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and organochlorine contaminants in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) from the Eastern Atlantic Ocean. Marine Environmental Research, 100, 48–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.marenvres.2014.03.010
Kremers, D., López Marulanda, J., Hausberger, M., & Lemasson, A. (2014). Behavioural evidence of magnetoreception in dolphins: detection of experimental magnetic fields. Naturwissenschaften, 101(11), 907–911. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-014-1231-x
García-Párraga, D., Brook, F., Crespo-Picazo, J. L., Alvaro, T., Valls, M., Penadés, M., … Corpa, J. M. (2014). Recurrent umbilical cord accidents in a bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus. Diseases of Aquatic Organisms, 108(2), 177–180. https://doi.org/10.3354/dao02711
Fabre, J. (2014). La communication acoustique chez le dauphin et l’impact de la pollution sonore.
García-Alvarez, N., Martín, V., Fernández, A., Almunia, J., Xuriach, A., Arbelo, M., … Luzardo, O. P. (2014). Levels and profiles of POPs (organochlorine pesticides, PCBs, and PAHs) in free-ranging common bottlenose dolphins of the Canary Islands, Spain. Science of the Total Environment, 493(July), 22–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2014.05.125
Kuczaj, S. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2014). Why do Dolphins Play? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2(2), 113–127. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.05.03.2014
Dold, C., & Ridgway, S. (2014). Cetaceans. In Gary West, D. Heard, & N. Caulkett (Eds.), Zoo Animal and Wildlife Immobilization and Anesthesia (John Wiley, p. 679:691).
Melero, M., García-Párraga, D., Corpa, J. M., Ortega, J., Rubio-Guerri, C., Crespo, J. L., … Sánchez-Vizcaíno, J. M. (2014). First molecular detection and characterization of herpesvirus and poxvirus in a Pacific walrus (Odobenus rosmarus divergens). BMC Veterinary Research, 10(1), 968. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-014-0308-2
Griffeth, R. J., García-Párraga, D., Mellado-López, M., Crespo-Picazo, J. L., Soriano-Navarro, M., Martinez-Romero, A., & Moreno-Manzano, V. (2014). Platelet-rich plasma and adipose-derived mesenchymal stem cells for regenerative medicine-associated treatments in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). PLoS ONE, 9(9).
Kuczaj II, S. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2014). The “creative dolphin” revisited: What do dolphins do when asked to vary their behavior? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(1). https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.02.05.2014
Rengifo-Herrera, C., Ferre, I., Ortega-Mora, L. M., Rojo-Montejo, S., García-Moreno, F. T., García-Párraga, D., … Pedraza-Díaz, S. (2014). Helminth parasites found in faecal samples of phocids from the Antarctic Peninsula. Polar Biology, 37(5), 685–695. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00300-014-1469-y
Kremers, D., Jaramillo, M. B., Böye, M., & Lemasson, A. (2014). Nocturnal Vocal Activity in Captive Bottlenose Dolphins ( Tursiops truncatus ): Could Dolphins have Presleep Choruses ? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1(4), 464–469. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.11.04.2014
García-Álvarez, N., Boada, L. D., Fernández, A., Zumbado, M., Arbelo, M., Sierra, E., … Luzardo, O. P. (2014). Assessment of the levels of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and organochlorine contaminants in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) from the Eastern Atlantic Ocean. Marine Environmental Research, 100, 48–56.
Bernal-Guadarrama, M. J., García-Parraga, D., Fernández-Gallardo, N., Zamora-Padrón, R., Pacheco, V., Reyes-Batlle, M., … Martínez-Carretero, E. (2014). Development of an indirect immunofluorescence technique for the evaluation of generated antibody titers against Erysipelothrix rhusiopathiae in captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Archives of Microbiology, 196(11), 785–790.
Formigaro, C., Henríquez-Hernandez, L. A., Zaccaroni, A., Garcia-Hartmann, M., Camacho, M., Boada, L. D., … Luzardo, O. P. (2014). Assessment of current dietary intake of organochlorine contaminants and polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons in killer whales (Orcinus orca) through direct determination in a group of whales in captivity. Science of the Total Environment, 472, 1044–1051.
Hymery, N., Alfonsi, E., Jamin, C., Dumas, C., Chambon, T., Hassani, S., & Jung, J.-L. (2013). Isolation of CD34 + cells from peripheral blood and bone marrow of Tursiops truncatus. Marine Mammal Science, 29(1), 195–203. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2011.00540.x
Kremers, D. (2013). Diving deeper into the dolphin’s Umwelt: Acoustic, gustatory, olfactory and magnetic perception. Rennes 1.
Favaro, L., Gnone, G., & Pessani, D. (2013). Postnatal Development of Echolocation Abilities in a Bottlenose Dolphin ( Tursiops truncatus ): Temporal Organization. Zoo Biology, 32(2), 210–215. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21056
Herman, L. M., Auersperg, A. M. I., von Bayern, A. M. P., Gajdon, G. K., Huber, L., Kacelnik, A., … Kuczaj, S. A. (2013). What Laboratory Research has Told Us about Dolphin Cognition. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(6), e20231. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21096
von Streit, C., Ganslosser, U., & von Fersen, L. (2013). Behavioral development of two captive mother-calf dyads of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the calves’ first year. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 26, 176–196.
Rubio-Guerri, C., Melero, M., Esperón, F., Bellière, E. N., Arbelo, M., Crespo, J. L., … Sánchez-Vizcaíno, J. M. (2013). Unusual striped dolphin mass mortality episode related to cetacean morbillivirus in the Spanish Mediterranean Sea. BMC Veterinary Research, 9(1), 106. https://doi.org/10.1186/1746-6148-9-106
Kim, S., Amundin, M., & Laska, M. (2013). Olfactory discrimination ability of South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus) for enantiomers. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 199(6), 535–544.
Ugaz, C., Valdez, R. A., Romano, M. C., & Galindo, F. (2013). Behavior and salivary cortisol of captive dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) kept in open and closed facilities. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 8(4). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2012.10.006
Venn-Watson, S. K., Jensen, E. D., Smith, C. R., Xitco, M., & Ridgway, S. H. (2013). Evaluation of annual survival and mortality rates and longevity of bottlenose dolphins Marine Mammal Program from 2004 through 2013. Aquatic Manimals, 246(8), 893–898. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.246.8.893
Marley, S. A., Cheney, B., & Thompson, P. M. (2013). Using tooth rakes to monitor population and sex differences in aggressive behaviour in bottlenose dolphins (tursiops truncatus). Aquatic Mammals, 39(2), 107–115. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.39.2.2013.107
Evans-Wilent, J., & Dudzinski, K. M. (2013). Vocalizations associated with pectoral fin contact in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Behavioural Processes, 100, 74–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2013.07.025
Kim, S., Amundin, M., & Laska, M. (2013). Olfactory discrimination ability of South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus) for enantiomers. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 199(6), 535–544.
Snowdon, C. T. (2013). Ah, sweet mystery of life, you still elude me. PsycCRITIQUES, 58(21). https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031011
Dudzinski, K. M., Gregg, J., Melillo-Sweeting, K., Seay, B., Levengood, A., & Kuczaj II, S. (2012). Tactile contact exchanges between dolphins : self-rubbing versus inter-individual contact in three species from three geographies. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 25, 21–43.
Tasker, M., Amundin, M., Andre, M., Hawkins, A. D., Lang, W., Merck, T., … Zakharia, M. (2012). Managing underwater noise in European waters: implementing the Marine Strategy Framework Directive. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, 730, 583–585.
Siniscalchi, M., Dimatteo, S., Pepe, A. M., Sasso, R., & Quaranta, A. (2012). Visual lateralization in wild striped dolphins (Stenella coeruleoalba) in response to stimuli with different degrees of familiarity. PLoS ONE, 7(1).
Tasker, M., Amundin, M., Michel, A., Hawkins, A. D., Lang, B., Merck, T., … Zakharia, M. (2012). Managing Underwater Noise in European Waters – Implementing the Marine Strategy Framework Directive. In A. Hawkins & A. N. Popper (Eds.), The Effects of Noise on Aquatic Life (pp. 583–589). Springer.
van Elk, C. E., Boelens, H. A. M., van Belkum, A., Foster, G., & Kuiken, T. (2012). Indications for both host-specific and introduced genotypes of Staphylococcus aureus in marine mammals. Veterinary Microbiology, 156(3–4), 343–346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vetmic.2011.10.034
Blois-Heulin, C., Crével, M., Böye, M., & Lemasson, A. (2012). Visual laterality in dolphins: importance of the familiarity of stimuli. BMC Neuroscience, 13(9). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-13-9
Kuczaj, S. A., Highfill, L. E., Makecha, R. N., & Byerly, H. C. (2012). Why Do Dolphins Smile? A Comparative Perspective on Dolphin Emotions and Emotional Expressions. In Science (pp. 63–85). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54123-3_4
Henderson, S. (2012). Abundance and survival of bottlenose Dolphins in Doubtful Sound and Dusky Sound – 2009-2012.
Brando, S., & Panksepp, J. (2012). The 7 emotional systems and their implications for captive wild animal wellbeing, (2004), 2012.
Liwanag, H. E. M., Berta, A., Costa, D. P., Budge, S. M., & Williams, T. M. (2012). Morphological and thermal properties of mammalian insulation: The evolutionary transition to blubber in pinnipeds. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 107(4), 774–787. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8312.2012.01992.x
Yaman, S., Kilian, A., von Fersen, L., & Güntürkün, O. (2012). Evidence for a numerosity category that is based on abstract qualities of “few” vs. “many” in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Frontiers in Psychology, 3(NOV).
Scharpegge, J., Hartmann, M. G., & Eulenberger, K. (2012). Thoracic auscultation in captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), California sea lions (Zalophus californianus), and South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus) with an electronic stethoscope. Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine : Official Publication of the American Association of Zoo Veterinarians, 43(2), 265–274. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22779229
Dudzinski, K. M., Gregg, J., Melillo-Sweeting, K., Seay, B., Levengood, A., & Kuczaj II, S. (2012). Tactile contact exchanges between dolphins : self-rubbing versus inter-individual contact in three species from three geographies. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 25, 21–43.
Herman, L. (2012). Historical Perspectives: Birthing a Dolphin Research Laboratory: The Early History of the Kewalo Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory. Aquatic Mammals, 38(1), 102–125. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.38.1.2012.102
Kuczaj, S. A., Highfill, L. E., Makecha, R. N., & Byerly, H. C. (2012). Why Do Dolphins Smile? A Comparative Perspective on Dolphin Emotions and Emotional Expressions. In Science (pp. 63–85). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54123-3_4
Kremers, D., Lemasson, A., Almunia, J., & Wanker, R. (2012). Vocal sharing and individual acoustic distinctiveness within a group of captive orcas (Orcinus orca). Journal of Comparative Psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983), 126(4), 433–445. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028858
Ridgway, S., Carder, D., Jeffries, M., & Todd, M. (2012). Spontaneous human speech mimicry by a cetacean. Current Biology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.044
Esperón, F., García-Párraga, D., Bellière, E. N., & Sánchez-Vizcaíno, J. M. (2012). Molecular diagnosis of lobomycosis-like disease in a bottlenose dolphin in captivity. Medical Mycology, 50(1), 106–109. https://doi.org/10.3109/13693786.2011.594100
Kremers, D., Jaramillo, M. B., Böye, M., Lemasson, A., & Hausberger, M. (2011). Do Dolphins Rehearse Show-Stimuli When at Rest? Delayed Matching of Auditory Memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00386
Wahlberg, M., Jensen, F. H., Aguilar Soto, N., Beedholm, K., Bejder, L., Oliveira, C., … Madsen, P. T. (2011). Source parameters of echolocation clicks from wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus and Tursiops truncatus). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
Perrin, W. F., Thieleking, J. L., Walker, W. A., Archer, F. I., & Robertson, K. M. (2011). Common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in California waters: Cranial differentiation of coastal and offshore ecotypes. Marine Mammal Science, 27(4), 769–792. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2010.00442.x
Starkhammar, J., Nilsson, J., Amundin, M., Kuczaj, S. A., Almqvist, M., & Persson, H. W. (2011). Separating overlapping click trains originating from multiple individuals in echolocation recordings. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(1), 458–466.
Starkhammar, J., Nilsson, J., Amundin, M., Kuczaj, S. A., Almqvist, M., & Persson, H. W. (2011). Separating overlapping click trains originating from multiple individuals in echolocation recordings. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(1), 458–466.
GENERAL FORMAT AND LITERATURE CITATION GUIDELINES FOR AQUATIC MAMMALS JOURNAL. (2011). Aquatic Mammals, (May), 1–14.
Siebert, U. (2011). Investigations of Thyroid and Stress Hormones in Free-Ranging and Captive Harbor Porpoises (Phocoena phocoena): A Pilot Study. Aquatic Mammals, 37(4), 443–453. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.37.4.2011.443
Greene, W. E., Project, D. C., Melillo-sweeting, K., & Dudzinski, K. M. (2011). Comparing Object Play in Captive and Wild Dolphins. International Journal, 24, 292–306. Retrieved from http://www.dolphincommunicationproject.org/pdf/Greene_etal_2011.pdf
Greene, W. E., Project, D. C., Melillo-sweeting, K., & Dudzinski, K. M. (2011). Comparing Object Play in Captive and Wild Dolphins. International Journal, 24, 292–306. Retrieved from http://www.dolphincommunicationproject.org/pdf/Greene_etal_2011.pdf
Lucke, K., Lepper, P. A., Blanchet, M.-A., & Siebert, U. (2011). The use of an air bubble curtain to reduce the received sound levels for harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(5), 3406. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3626123
Melero, M., Rubio-Guerri, C., Crespo, J. L., Arbelo, M., Vela, A. I., Garc??a-P??rraga, D., … S??nchez-Vizca??no, J. M. (2011). First case of erysipelas in a free-ranging bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) stranded in the Mediterranean Sea. Diseases of Aquatic Organisms, 97(2), 167–170. https://doi.org/10.3354/dao02412
Thieltges, H., Lemasson, A., Kuczaj, S., Böye, M., & Blois-Heulin, C. (2011). Visual laterality in dolphins when looking at (un)familiar humans. Animal Cognition, 14(2), 303–308. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0354-5
Rengifo-Herrera, C., Ortega-Mora, L. M., G??mez-Bautista, M., Garc??a-Moreno, F. T., Garc??a-P??rraga, D., Castro-Urda, J., & Pedraza-D??az, S. (2011). Detection and characterization of a Cryptosporidium isolate from a southern elephant seal (Mirounga leonina) from the Antarctic Peninsula. Applied and Environmental Microbiology, 77(4), 1524–1527. https://doi.org/10.1128/AEM.01422-10
Kremers, D., Jaramillo, M. B., Böye, M., Lemasson, A., & Hausberger, M. (2011). Do dolphins rehearse show-stimuli when at rest? Delayed matching of auditory memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(December), 386. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00386
Delfour, F. (2010). Marine Mammals Enact Individual Worlds. International Journal, 23(1), 792–810. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebdp.2005.01.005
Cranford, T. W., Krysl, P., & Amundin, M. (2010). A new acoustic portal into the odontocete ear and vibrational analysis of the tympanoperiotic complex. PloS One, 5(8), e11927. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0011927
Horback, K. M., Friedman, W. R., Johnson, C. M., & San, C. (2010). The Occurrence and Context of S-Posture Display by Captive Belugas ( Delphinapterus leucas ). International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 689–700.
Yeater, D. B., & Kuczaj II, S. a. (2010). Observational Learning in Wild and Captive Dolphins. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(3), 379–385.
Dudzinski, K. M. (2010). Overlap between Information Gained from Complementary and Comparative Studies of Captive and Wild Dolphins. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(2), 566–586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebdp.2005.04.004
Garcia-Pena, F. J., Perez-Boto, D., Jimenez, C., San Miguel, E., Echeita, A., Rengifo-Herrera, C., … Pedraza-Diaz, S. (2010). Isolation and Characterization of Campylobacter spp. from Antarctic Fur Seals (Arctocephalus gazella) at Deception Island, Antarctica. Applied and Environmental Microbiology, 76(17), 6013–6016. https://doi.org/10.1128/AEM.00316-10
Biancani, B., Lacave, G., Magi, G. E., & Rossi, G. (2010). Ovarian interstitial cell tumor in a South American sea lion (Otaria flavescens). Journal of Wildlife Diseases, 46(3), 1012–1016. https://doi.org/10.7589/0090-3558-46.3.1012
Brando, S. I. C. A. (2010). Advances in Husbandry Training in Marine Mammal Care Programs. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(1), 777–791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebdp.2005.01.008
Mercado III, E., & DeLong, C. M. (2010). Dolphin Cognition : Representations and Processes in Memory and Perception. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 344–378. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004
Laska, M., Lord, E., Selin, S., & Amundin, M. (2010). Olfactory discrimination of aliphatic odorants in South African fur seals (arctocephalus pusillus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 124(2), 187–193. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018189
Tomonaga, M. (2010). Bottlenose Dolphins’ (Tursiops truncatus) Theory of Mind as Demonstrated by Responses to their Trainers’ Attentional States. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 386–400. Retrieved from http://www.comparativepsychology.org/ijcp-2010-3/10.Tomonaga_etal_FINAL.pdf
López-Rivas, R. M., & Bazúa-Durán, C. (2010). Who is whistling? Localizing and identifying phonating dolphins in captivity. Applied Acoustics, 71(11), 1057–1062. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apacoust.2010.05.006
Tasker, M. L., Amundin, M., Andre, M., Hawkins, A., Lang, W., & Merck, T. (2010). Marine Strategy Framework Directive: Underwater noise and other forms of energy. JRC Scientific and Technical Reports. https://doi.org/10.2788/87079
Gerber, L. R., González-Suárez, M., Hernández-Camacho, C. J., Young, J. K., & Sabo, J. L. (2010). The Cost of Male Aggression and Polygyny in California Sea Lions (Zalophus californianus). PLoS ONE, 5(8), e12230. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012230
Delfour, F. (2010). Marine Mammals Enact Individual Worlds. International Journal, 23(1), 792–810. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebdp.2005.01.005
Cranford, T. W., Krysl, P., & Amundin, M. (2010). A new acoustic portal into the odontocete ear and vibrational analysis of the tympanoperiotic complex. PloS One, 5(8), e11927. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0011927
Luke, J. P., Marichal-Hernandez, J. G., Rosa, F., & Almunia, J. (2010). Real time automatic detection of Orcinus orca vocalizations in a controlled environment. Applied Acoustics, 71(8), 771–776.
Dudzinski, K. M. (2010). Overlap between Information Gained from Complementary and Comparative Studies of Captive and Wild Dolphins. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(2), 566–586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebdp.2005.04.004
Rehtanz, M., Ghim, S.-J., McFee, W., Doescher, B., Lacave, G., Fair, P. a, … Jenson, A. B. (2010). Papillomavirus antibody prevalence in free-ranging and captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Journal of Wildlife Diseases, 46(1), 136–145. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20090026
Lucke, K., Siebert, U., Lepper, P. A., & Blanchet, M.-A. (2009). Temporary shift in masked hearing thresholds in a harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) after exposure to seismic airgun stimuli. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(6), 4060. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3117443
Lucke, K., Siebert, U., Lepper, P. A., & Blanchet, M.-A. (2009). Temporary shift in masked hearing thresholds in a harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) after exposure to seismic airgun stimuli. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(6), 4060. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3117443
Perrin, W. F., Würsig, B., & Thewissen, J. G. M. (Eds.). (2009). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals (Vol. 53).
Starkhammar, J., Amundin, M., Nilsson, J., Jansson, T., Kuczaj, S. A., Almqvist, M., & Persson, H. W. (2009). 47-channel burst-mode recording hydrophone system enabling measurements of the dynamic echolocation behavior of free-swimming dolphins. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126(3), 959–962.
Dudzinski, K. M., Gregg, J. D., Ribic, C. A., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2009). A comparison of pectoral fin contact between two different wild dolphin populations. Behavioural Processes, 80(2), 182–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2008.11.011
Esch, H. C., Sayigh, L. S., Blum, J. E., & Wells, R. S. (2009). Whistles as Potential Indicators of Stress in Bottlenose Dolphins ( Tursiops truncatus ). Journal of Mammalogy, 90(3), 638–650. https://doi.org/10.1644/08-MAMM-A-069R.1
Kuczaj II, S. A., & Thames Walker, R. (2009). How Do Dolphins Solve Problems? In Comparative CognitionExperimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence (pp. 580–601). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195377804.003.0030
Wells, R. S. (2009). Learning from nature: Bottlenose dolphin care and husbandry. Zoo Biology, 28(6), 635–651. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20252
Starkhammar, J., Amundin, M., Nilsson, J., Jansson, T., Kuczaj, S. A., Almqvist, M., & Persson, H. W. (2009). 47-channel burst-mode recording hydrophone system enabling measurements of the dynamic echolocation behavior of free-swimming dolphins. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126(3), 959–962.
Ridgway, S., Keogh, M., Carder, D., Finneran, J., Kamolnick, T., Todd, M., & Goldblatt, A. (2009). Dolphins maintain cognitive performance during 72 to 120 hours of continuous auditory vigilance. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 212(Pt 10), 1519–1527. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.027896
Kuczaj II, S. A., & Thames Walker, R. (2009). How Do Dolphins Solve Problems? In E. A. Wasserman & T. R. Zentall (Eds.), Comparative CognitionExperimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence (Oxford Uni, pp. 580–601). New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195377804.003.0030
Melillo, K. E., Dudzinski, K. M., & Cornick, L. A. (2009). Interactions Between Atlantic Spotted (Stenella frontalis) and Bottlenose (Tursiops truncatus) Dolphins off Bimini, The Bahamas, 2003-2007. Aquatic Mammals, 35(2), 281–291. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.35.2.2009.281
Arcangeli, a, & Crosti, R. (2009). The short-term impact of dolphin-watching on the behaviour of bottlenose dolphins(Tursiops truncatus) in western Australia. Journal of Marine Animals and Their Ecology, 2(1), 3–9.
Kuczaj II, S. A., & Thames Walker, R. (2009). How Do Dolphins Solve Problems? In E. A. Wasserman & T. R. Zentall (Eds.), Comparative CognitionExperimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence (Oxford Uni, pp. 580–601). New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195377804.003.0030
Dudzinski, K. M., Gregg, J. D., Ribic, C. A., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2009). A comparison of pectoral fin contact between two different wild dolphin populations. Behavioural Processes, 80(2), 182–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2008.11.011
Parsons, K. M., Balcomb, K. C., Ford, J. K. B., & Durban, J. W. (2009). The social dynamics of southern resident killer whales and conservation implications for this endangered population. Animal Behaviour, 77, 963–971. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.018
Melillo, K. E., Dudzinski, K. M., & Cornick, L. a. (2009). Interactions Between Atlantic Spotted (<I>Stenella frontalis</I>) and Bottlenose (<I>Tursiops truncatus</I>) Dolphins off Bimini, The Bahamas, 2003-2007. Aquatic Mammals, 35(2), 281–291. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.35.2.2009.281
Weijs, L., Das, K., Siebert, U., van Elk, N., Jauniaux, T., Neels, H., … Covaci, A. (2009). Concentrations of chlorinated and brominated contaminants and their metabolites in serum of harbour seals and harbour porpoises. Environment International, 35(6), 842–850.
Dudzinski, K. M., Gregg, J. D., Ribic, C. A., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2009). A comparison of pectoral fin contact between two different wild dolphin populations. Behavioural Processes, 80(2), 182–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2008.11.011
Virenque, M. C., & Verwaerde, M. P. (2008). PROTOCOLES D ’ ANESTHESIE ET DE REANIMATION DES MAMMIFERES MARINS : APPLICATION AU GRAND DAUPHIN ( Tursiops truncatus ) ET A L ’ OTARIE DE CALIFORNIE ( Zalophus californianus ).
Starkhammar, J., Kuczaj, S. A., Amundin, M., Nilsson, J., Almqvist, M., & Persson, H. W. (2008). The dynamics of the bottlenose dolphin sonar beam. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(4), 2506. Retrieved from http://link.aip.org/link/?JASMAN/124/2506/3
Paulos, R. D., Dudzinski, K. M., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2008). The role of touch in select social interactions of Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis) and Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops aduncus). Journal of Ethology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-007-0047-y
Hill, H. M., Carder, D. A., Ridgway, S. H., & San, C. (2008). Vigilance in Female Bottlenose Dolphins ( Tursiops sp .) Before and After Calving. International Journal, 21(1), 35–57. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/6pr5k7b4.pdf
Lucke, K., Lepper, P. A., Blanchet, M.-A., & Siebert, U. (2008). Testing the acoustic tolerance of harbour porpoise hearing for impulsive sounds. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(5), 3780. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2935423
Amundin, M., Starkhammar, J., Evander, M., Almqvist, M., Lindström, K., & Persson, H. W. (2008). An echolocation visualization and interface system for dolphin research. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(2), 1188–1194. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18247918
Young, J. K., González-suárez, M., & Gerber, L. R. (2008). Determinants of agonistic interactions in California sea lions. Wildlife Conservation, (May), 1797–1810.
Kuczaj, S., Solangi, M., Hoffland, T., Romagnoli, M., & Mammal, M. (2008). Recognition and Discrimination of Human Actions Across the Senses of Echolocation and Vision in the Bottlenose Dolphin : Evidence for Dolphin Cross-modal Integration of Dynamic Information. International Journal, 21(2), 84–95. Retrieved from http://www2.gsu.edu/~wwwscp/ijcp-vo21-2-2008/Kuczaj_Final_pdf.pdf
Torno, C. S., Buccat, M. C., & Masangkay, J. S. (2008). Aspergillosis in a melon-headed whale (peponocephala electra). Philippine Journal of Veterinary Medicine, 45(1), 49–57.
Laska, M., Svelander, M., & Amundin, M. (2008). Successful acquisition of an olfactory discrimination paradigm by South African fur seals, Arctocephalus pusillus. Physiology & Behavior, 93(4–5), 1033–1038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.01.019
Laska, M., Svelander, M., & Amundin, M. (2008). Successful acquisition of an olfactory discrimination paradigm by South African fur seals, Arctocephalus pusillus. Physiology & Behavior, 93(4–5), 1033–1038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.01.019
Lyamin, O. I., Manger, P. R., Ridgway, S. H., Mukhametov, L. M., & Siegel, J. M. (2008). Cetacean sleep: an unusual form of mammalian sleep. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(8), 1451–1484. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.023
Bien-, C. D. U., & Animaux, T. R. E. D. E. S. (2008). La détention de rapaces en captivité chez les particuliers.
Amundin, M., Starkhammar, J., Evander, M., Almqvist, M., Lindström, K., & Persson, H. W. (2008). An echolocation visualization and interface system for dolphin research. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(2), 1188–1194. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18247918
Lucke, K., Lepper, P. A., Blanchet, M.-A., Hoeve, B., Everaarts, E., van Elk, N., & Siebert, U. (2008). Auditory studies on harbour porpoises in relation to offshore wind turbines. In P. G. H. Evans (Ed.), 15th ASCOBANS Advisory Committee Meeting (Vol. Special Pu, pp. 1–4).
Lucke, K., Lepper, P. A., Blanchet, M.-A., Hoeve, B., Everaarts, E., van Elk, N., & Siebert, U. (2008). Auditory studies on harbour porpoises in relation to offshore wind turbines. In P. G. H. Evans (Ed.), 15th ASCOBANS Advisory Committee Meeting (Vol. Special Pu, pp. 1–4).
Noren, S. R. (2008). Infant carrying behaviour in dolphins: costly parental care in an aquatic environment. Functional Ecology, 22(2), 284–288. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2007.01354.x
Lucke, K., Lepper, P. A., Blanchet, M.-A., & Siebert, U. (2008). Testing the acoustic tolerance of harbour porpoise hearing for impulsive sounds. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(5), 3780. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2935423
Starkhammar, J., Kuczaj, S. A., Amundin, M., Nilsson, J., Almqvist, M., & Persson, H. W. (2008). The dynamics of the bottlenose dolphin sonar beam. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(4), 2506. Retrieved from http://link.aip.org/link/?JASMAN/124/2506/3
Jacobs, D. F., Hernandez-Camacho, C. J., Young, J. K., & Gerber, L. R. (2008). Determinants of outcomes of agonistic interactions among male california sea lions (Zalophus californianus). Journal of Mammalogy, 89(5), 1212–1217.
Lucke, K., Lepper, P. A., Blanchet, M., & Siebert, U. (2007). Tolerance of harbour porpoise hearing to single airgun impulses Ramming of foundation. Components, (November).
Kuczaj, S., Wright, A. J., Highfill, L., & Editors, G. (2007). Considerations of the Effects of Noise on Marine Mammals and other Animals Andrew. International Journal of Comparative Psychology (Vol. 20).
Harper, C. (2007). Morphology of the Melon and Its Tendinous Connections To the. Journal of Morphology.
Highfill, L. E., & Kuczaj II, S. A. (2007). Do Bottlenose Dolphins (<I>Tursiops truncatus</I>) Have Distinct and Stable Personalities? Aquatic Mammals, 33(3), 380–389. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.33.3.2007.380
Nachtigall, P. E., Supin, A. Y., Amundin, M., Roken, B., Moller, T., Mooney, T. A., … Yuen, M. (2007). Polar bear Ursus maritimus hearing measured with auditory evoked potentials. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210(7), 1116–1122. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.02734
Lucke, K., Lepper, P. A., Blanchet, M., & Siebert, U. (2007). Tolerance of harbour porpoise hearing to single airgun impulses Ramming of foundation. Components, (November).
Kastelein, R. A., Jennings, N., & Postma, J. (2007). Feeding enrichment methods for Pacific walrus calves. Zoo Biology, 26(3), 175–186.
Starkhammar, J., Amundin, M., Olsén, H., Ahlmqvist, M., Lindström, K., & Persson, H. W. (2007). Acoustic touch screen for dolphins first application of ELVIS – An echo-location visualization and interface system. In Proceedings of the Institute of Acoustics (Vol. 29, pp. 63–68). Retrieved from https://lup.lub.lu.se/search/publication/768807%5Cnhttp://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?eid=2-s2.0-38849126567&partnerID=tZOtx3y1
Santos, M. B., Fernández, R., López, A., Martínez, J. A., & Pierce, G. J. (2007). Variability in the diet of bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus, in Galician waters, north-western Spain, 1990–2005. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the UK, 87(01), 231. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025315407055233
Bonar, C. J., Boede, E. O., Hartmann, M. G., Lowenstein-Whaley, J., Mujica-Jorquera, E., Parish, S. V, … Stadler, C. K. (2007). A retrospective study of pathologic findings in the Amazon and Orinoco river dolphin (Inia geoffrensis) in captivity. Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine : Official Publication of the American Association of Zoo Veterinarians, 38(2), 177–191.
Starkhammar, J., Amundin, M., Olsén, H., Ahlmqvist, M., Lindström, K., & Persson, H. W. (2007). Acoustic touch screen for dolphins first application of ELVIS – An echo-location visualization and interface system. In Proceedings of the Institute of Acoustics (Vol. 29, pp. 63–68). Retrieved from https://lup.lub.lu.se/search/publication/768807%255Cnhttp://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?eid=2-s2.0-38849126567&partnerID=tZOtx3y1
Gregg, J. D., Dudzinski, K. M., & Smith, H. V. (2007). Do dolphins eavesdrop on the echolocation signals of conspecifics? International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 20, 65–88.
Gregg, J. D., Dudzinski, K. M., & Smith, H. V. (2007). Do dolphins eavesdrop on the echolocation signals of conspecifics? International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 20, 65–88.
Paulos, R. D., Dudzinski, K. M., & Kuczaj, S. a. (2007). The role of touch in select social interactions of Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis) and Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops aduncus). Journal of Ethology, 26(1), 153–164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-007-0047-y
Hill, H., Greer, T., Solangi, M., & Kucjaz, S. A. (2007). All mothers are not the same: Maternal styles in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). International Journal of …, 20(1), 35–54. Retrieved from http://escholarship.org/uc/item/0390q1j9.pdf
Lucke, K., Lepper, P. a., Hoeve, B., Everaarts, E., van Elk, N., & Siebert, U. (2007). Perception of Low-Frequency Acoustic Signals by a Harbour Porpoise (<I>Phocoena phocoena</I>) in the Presence of Simulated Offshore Wind Turbine Noise. Aquatic Mammals, 33(1), 55–68. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.33.1.2007.55
Desportes, G., Buholzer, L., Anderson-Hansen, K., Blanchet, M.-A., Acquarone, M., Shephard, G., … Siebert, U. (2007). Decrease stress; train your animals: The effect of handling methods on cortisol levels in harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) under human care. Aquatic Mammals, 33(3), 286–292. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.33.3.2007.286
Spitz, J., Rousseau, Y., & Ridoux, V. (2006). Diet overlap between harbour porpoise and bottlenose dolphin: An argument in favour of interference competition for food? Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science, 70(1–2), 259–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecss.2006.04.020
Dudzinski, K. M., Thomas, J. A., & Gregg, J. D. (2006). Communication in Marine Mammals. Developmental Psychology, (1990), 260–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-373553-9.00064-X
Ridgway, S., Carder, D., Finneran, J., Keogh, M., Kamolnick, T., Todd, M., & Goldblatt, A. (2006). Dolphin continuous auditory vigilance for five days. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209(Pt 18), 3621–3628. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16943502
Garcia Hartmann, M., Benvenuti, A., Stanzani, G., Fichtel, L., Scharpegge, J., & Bianchani, B. (2006). Rehabilitation And Management Of A Risso’s Dolphin’s (Grampus Griseus) Calf: some veterinary aspects. In Proceedings of the 34th Symposium of the European Association for Aquatic Mammals, Riccione, Italy, 17-21 March 2006 (pp. 1–10). European Association for Aquatic Mammals.
Dudzinski, K. M., Thomas, J. A., & Gregg, J. D. (2006). Communication in Marine Mammals. Developmental Psychology, (1990), 260–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-373553-9.00064-X
Kuczaj, S. a., & Yeater, D. B. (2006). Dolphin Imitation: Who, What, When, and Why? Aquatic Mammals, 32(4), 413–422. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.32.4.2006.413
Delfour, F., & Marten, K. (2006). Lateralized visual behavior in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) performing audio-visual tasks: The right visual field advantage. Behavioural Processes, 71(1), 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.09.005
Kuczaj, S. a., & Yeater, D. B. (2006). Dolphin Imitation: Who, What, When, and Why? Aquatic Mammals, 32(4), 413–422. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.32.4.2006.413
Böye, M., Güntürkün, O., & Vauclair, J. (2005). Right ear advantage for conspecific calls in adults and subadults, but not infants, California sea lions ( Zalophus californianus ): hemispheric specialization for communication? European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(6), 1727–1732. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04005.x
Blomqvist, C., Mello, I., & Amundin, M. (2005). An Acoustic Play-Fight Signal in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Human Care. Aquatic Mammals, 31(2), 187–194. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.31.2.2005.187
Mello, I., & Amundin, M. (2005). Whistle Production Pre- and Post-Partum in Bottlenose Dolphins (<I>Tursiops truncatus</I>) in Human Care. Aquatic Mammals, 31(2), 169–175. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.31.2.2005.169
Kuczaj, S. A. I. I., & Highfill, L. E. (2005). Dolphin play: Evidence for cooperation and culture? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 705–706. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05370129
Kilian, A., Fersen, L. von, & Güntürkün, O. (2005). Left hemispheric advantage for numerical abilities in the bottlenose dolphin. Behavioural Processes, 68(2), 179–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2004.11.003
Kilian, A., Fersen, L. von, & Güntürkün, O. (2005). Left hemispheric advantage for numerical abilities in the bottlenose dolphin. Behavioural Processes, 68(2), 179–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2004.11.003
Kuczaj, S. A. I. I., & Highfill, L. E. (2005). Dolphin play: Evidence for cooperation and culture? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 705–706. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05370129
Mello, I., & Amundin, M. (2005). Whistle Production Pre- and Post-Partum in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops Truncatus) in Human Care. Aquatic Mammals, 31(2), 0–7. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.31.2.2005.169
Blomqvist, C., Mello, I., & Amundin, M. (2005). An Acoustic Play-Fight Signal in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Human Care. Aquatic Mammals, 31(2), 187–194. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.31.2.2005.187
Silva-jr, J. M., Silva, F. J. L., & Sazima, I. (2005). Rest , nurture , sex , release , and play : diurnal underwater behaviour of the spinner dolphin at Fernando de Noronha Archipelago , SW Atlantic. Aqua, Journal of Ichthyology and Aquatic Biology, 9(4), 161–176.
Böye, M., Güntürkün, O., & Vauclair, J. (2005). Right ear advantage for conspecific calls in adults and subadults, but not infants, California sea lions ( Zalophus californianus ): hemispheric specialization for communication? European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(6), 1727–1732. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04005.x
Blomqvist, C., Mello, I., & Amundin, M. (2005). An Acoustic Play-Fight Signal in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Human Care. Aquatic Mammals, 31(2), 187–194. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.31.2.2005.187
Mello, I., Nordensten, L., & Amundin, M. (2005). Reactions of three bottlenose dolphin dams with calves to other members of the group in connection with nursing. Zoo Biology, 24(6), 543–555.
Mello, I., Nordensten, L., & Amundin, M. (2005). Reactions of three bottlenose dolphin dams with calves to other members of the group in connection with nursing. Zoo Biology, 24(6), 543–555.
Mello, I., & Amundin, M. (2005). Whistle Production Pre- and Post-Partum in Bottlenose Dolphins (<I>Tursiops truncatus</I>) in Human Care. Aquatic Mammals, 31(2), 169–175. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.31.2.2005.169
Lacave, G., Eggermont, M., Verslycke, T., Brook, F., Salbany, A., Roque, L., & Kinoshita, R. (2004). Prediction from ultrasonographic measurements of the expected delivery date in two species of bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus and Tursiops aduncus). The Veterinary Record, 154, 228–233.
Hendry, J. L. (2004). The ontogeny of echolocation in the atlantic bottlenose dolphin, 1–133.
Blomqvist, C., & Amundin, M. (2004). An Acoustic Tag for Recording Directional Pulsed Ultrasounds Aimed at Free-Swimming Bottlenose Dolphins (<I>Tursiops truncatus</I>) by Conspecifics. Aquatic Mammals, 30(3), 345–356. https://doi.org/10.1578/AM.30.3.2004.345
Dierking, L. D., Adelman, L. M., Ogden, J., Lehnhardt, K., Miller, L., & Mellen, J. D. (2004). Using a behavior change model to document the impact of visits to Disney’s Animal Kingdom: A study investigating intended conservation action. Curator: The Museum Journal, 47(3), 322–343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2151-6952.2004.tb00128.x
Beineke, A., Siebert, U., van Elk, N., & Baumgärtner, W. (2004). Development of a lymphocyte-transformation-assay for peripheral blood lymphocytes of the harbor porpoise and detection of cytokines using the reverse-transcription polymerase chain reaction. Veterinary Immunology and Immunopathology, 98(1–2), 59–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vetimm.2003.10.002
Kilian, A., Von Fersen, L., & Güntürkün, O. (2004). The number sense of the bottlenose dolphin. European Cetacean Society 18th Annual Conference Experimental Approaches to Marine Mammal Research.
Geertsen, B. M., Teilmann, J., Kastelein, R. a, Vlemmix, H. N. J., & Miller, L. a. (2004). Behaviour and physiological effects of transmitter attachments on a captive harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena). Journal of Cetacean Research and Management, 6(2), 139–146.
Blomqvist, C., & Amundin, M. (2004). High-Frequency Burst-Pulse Sounds in Agonistic / Aggressive Interactions in Bottlenose Dolphins , Tursiops truncatus. Interactions, (1983), 425–431.
Kilian, A., Von Fersen, L., & Güntürkün, O. (2004). The number sense of the bottlenose dolphin. European Cetacean Society 18th Annual Conference Experimental Approaches to Marine Mammal Research.
Kilian, A., Yaman, S., Von Fersen, L., & Güntürkün, O. (2003). A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity. Learning Behavior a Psychonomic Society Publication, 31(2), 133–142. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12882372
Stolen, M. K., & Barlow, J. A. Y. (2003). A model life table for bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) from the indian river lagoon system, Florida, U.S.A. Marine Mammal Science, 19(4), 630–649.
Manger, P. R., Ridgway, S. H., & Siegel, J. M. (2003). The locus coeruleus complex of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) as revealed by tyrosine hydroxylase immunohistochemistry. Journal of Sleep Research, 12(2), 149–155. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12753352
Lusseau, D. (2003). Male and female bottlenose dolphins Tursiops spp. have different strategies to avoid interactions with tour boats in Doubtful Sound, New Zealand. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 257, 267–274. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps257267
Dudzinski, K. M., Sakai, M., Masaki, K., Kogi, K., Hishii, T., & Kurimoto, M. (2003). Behavioral observations of adult and sub-adult dolphins towards two dead bottlenose dolphins. Aquatic Mammals, 29(1), 108–116.
Dudzinski, K. M., Sakai, M., Masaki, K., Kogi, K., Hishii, T., & Kurimoto, M. (2003). Behavioural observations of bottlenose dolphins towards two dead conspecifics. Aquatic Mammals, 29(1), 108–116. https://doi.org/10.1578/016754203101023951
Dudzinski, K. M., Sakai, M., Masaki, K., Kogi, K., Hishii, T., & Kurimoto, M. (2003). Behavioral observations of adult and sub-adult dolphins towards two dead bottlenose dolphins. Aquatic Mammals, 29(1), 108–116.
Jacobsen, T. B., Mayntz, M., & Amundin, M. (2003). Splitting suckling data of bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) neonates in human care into suckling bouts. Zoo Biology, 22(5), 477–488.
Jacobsen, T. B., Mayntz, M., & Amundin, M. (2003). Splitting suckling data of bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) neonates in human care into suckling bouts. Zoo Biology, 22(5), 477–488.
Kilian, A., Yaman, S., Von Fersen, L., & Güntürkün, O. (2003). A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity. Learning Behavior a Psychonomic Society Publication, 31(2), 133–142. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12882372
Desportes, G., Kristensen, J. H., Benham, D., Wilson, S., Jepsen, T., Korsgaard, B., … Shephard, G. (2003). Multiple insights into the reproductive function of harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena): An ongoing study. NAMMCO Scientific Publications, 5, 91. https://doi.org/10.7557/3.2741
Parsons, K. M., Durban, J. W., & Claridge, D. E. (2003). Male-male aggression renders bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) unconscious. Aquatic Mammals, 29(3), 360–362. https://doi.org/10.1578/01675420360736532
Lacave, G., Eggermont, M., & Brook, F. (2002). Delivery Prediction Program in Tursiops truncatus Based on Ultrasound Measurements. International Association for Aquatic Animal Medicine, (May 2002), 97–99. Retrieved from http://www.vin.com/Proceedings/Proceedings.plx?&CID=IAAAM2002&PID=pr49892&O=Generic
Ulrike Griebel. (2002). Color Vision in Marine Mammals: A Review. In M. Bright, P. C. Dworschak, & M. Stachowitsch (Eds.), The Vienna School of Marine Biology: A Tribute to Jörg Ott. (Facultas U, p. 73:87).
Lacave, G., Salbany, A., & Roque, L. (2002). Twin Gestation in a Tursiops truncatus at Zoomarine , Portugal. International Association for Aquatic Animal Medicine, (May 2002), 100. Retrieved from http://www.vin.com/Proceedings/Proceedings.plx?CID=IAAAM2002&PID=49885&Print=1&O=Generic
Kastelein, R. A., Mosterd, P., Van Santen, B., Hagedoorn, M., & De Haan, D. (2002). Underwater audiogram of a Pacific walrus (Odobenus rosmarus divergens) measured with narrow-band frequency-modulated signals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 112(5), 2173. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1508783
Scotia, T. O. N. (2002). SPATIOTEMPORAL PREDICTION MODELS OF CETACEAN HABITATS I N THE MID-WESTERN NORTH ATLANTIC OCEAN ( FROM CAPE HATTERAS , NORTH CAROLINA , U . S . A . Marine Mammal Science, 18(October), 920–939.
Kuczaj, S., Lacinak, T., Otto, F., Trone, M., Solangi, M., & Ramos, J. (2002). Keeping environmental enrichment enriching. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 15(2), 127–137. Retrieved from http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/1bj376tj?display=all
Dehnhardt, G., Mauck, B., Hanke, W., & Bleckmann, H. (2001). Hydrodynamic trail-following in harbor seals (Phoca vitulina). Science, 293(5527), 102–104. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11441183
Lacave, G., Cox, E., Hermans, J., Devriese, L., & Goddeeris, B. M. (2001). Induction of cross-protection in mice against dolphin Erysipelothrix rhusiopathiae isolates with a swine commercial vaccine. Veterinary Microbiology, 80(3), 247–253.
Killebrew, D., Mercado, E., Herman, L., & Pack, A. (2001). Sound production of a neonate bottlenose dolphin. Aquatic Mammals, 27(1), 34–44. Retrieved from http://www.acsu.buffalo.edu/~emiii/aqmam01.PDF
Kastelein, R. A., Au, W. W., & de Haan, D. (2000). Detection distances of bottom-set gillnets by harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) and bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Marine Environmental Research, 49(4), 359–375. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11285736
Servais, V. (2000). Construire l’esprit du dauphin. Terrain, 34.
Barros, N. B., Parsons, E. C. M., & Jefferson, T. a. (2000). Prey of off shore bottlenose dolphins from the South China Sea. Aquatic Mammals, 26(1), 2–6.
Samuels, A., Bejder, L., & Heinrich, S. (2000). A Review of the Literature Pertaining to Swimming with Wild Dolphins. Conservation Biology.
Au, W. W. L., Popper, A. N., & Fay, R. R. (2000). Hearing by Whales and Dolphins. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, 12(January 2000), 485. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-1150-1
von Fersen, L., & Delius, J. D. (2000). Acquired equivalence between auditory stimuli in dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Cognition, 3, 79–83.
Kilian, A., Von Fersen, L., & Güntürkün, O. (2000). Lateralization of visuospatial processing in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Behavioural Brain Research, 116(2), 211–215. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11080552
Kilian, A., Von Fersen, L., & Güntürkün, O. (2000). Lateralization of visuospatial processing in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Behavioural Brain Research, 116(2), 211–215. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11080552
von Fersen, L., Schall, U., & Güntürkün, O. (2000). Visual lateralization of pattern discrimination in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Behavioural Brain Research, 107(1–2), 177–181. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10628742
McCowan, B., Marino, L., Vance, E., Walke, L., & Reiss, D. (2000). Bubble ring play of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Implications for cognition. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114(1), 98–106. https://doi.org/10.1037//0735-7036.114.1.98
Au, W. W., Kastelein, R. A., Rippe, T., & Schooneman, N. M. (1999). Transmission beam pattern and echolocation signals of a harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106(6), 3699–3705. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10615708
Baumgartner, M. F., Mullin, K. D., May, L. N., & Leming, T. D. (1999). Cetacean habitats in the northern Gulf of Mexico. Fisheries Bulletin, II, 219–239.
Miokovi, D., Kovacic, D., & Pribanic, S. (1999). STOMACH CONTENT ANALYSIS OF ONE BOTTLENOSE DOLPHIN (TURSIOPS TRUNCATUS , MONTAGUE 1821 ) FROM THE ADRIATIC SEA. Nat. Croat., 8(1), 61–65.
Gubbins, C., McOwan, B., Lynn, S. K., Hooper, S., & Reiss, D. (1999). Mother-Infant Spatial Relations In Captive Bottlenose Dolphins, Tursiops Truncatus. Marine Mammal Science, 15(3), 751–765. https://doi.org/doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.1999.tb00841.x
Wang, D. (1999). New Stereo Acoustic Data Logger for Free-ranging, 3–9.
Yazdi, P., Kilian, A., & Culik, B. M. (1999). Energy expenditure of swimming bottlenose dolphins ( Tursiops truncatus ). Marine Biology, 134(4), 601–607. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002270050575
Williams, T. M., Haun, J. E., & Friedl, W. A. (1999). The diving physiology of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) – I. Balancing the demands of exercise for energy conservation at depth. Journal of Experimental Biology, 202(20), 2739–2748.
Almunia, J., Basterretxea, G., Arı́stegui, J., & Ulanowicz, R. E. (1999). Benthic-Pelagic Switching in a Coastal Subtropical Lagoon. Estuarine, Coastal and Shelf Science, 49(3), 363–384. Retrieved from http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0272771499905036
Dudzinski, K. M. (1998). Contact behavior and signal exchange among Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis), 24(3), 129–142.
Dehnhardt, G., Mauck, B., & Hyvärinen, H. (1998). Ambient temperature does not affect the tactile sensitivity of mystacial vibrissae in harbour seals. Journal of Experimental Biology, 201(Pt 22), 3023–3029. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9787122
Dudzinski, K. M. (1998). Contact behavior and signal exchange among Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis). Aquatic Mammals, 24(3), 129–142.
Fernandez, S., & Hohn, A. A. (1998). Age, growth, and calving season of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus off coastal Texas. Fishery Bulletin.
Dudzinski, K. M. (1998). Contact behavior and signal exchange in Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis). Aquatic Mammals.
Patterson, I. A. P., Reid, R. J., Wilson, B., Grellier, K., Ross, H. M., & Thompson, P. M. (1998). Evidence for infanticide in bottlenose dolphins: an explanation for violent interactions with harbour porpoises? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265(1402), 1167–1170. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0414
Davis, R. W., Fargion, G. S., May, N., Leming, T. D., & Baumgartner, M. (1998). PHYSICAL HABITAT OF CETACEANS ALONG THE CONTINENTAL SLOPE IN THE NORTH- CENTRAL AND WESTERN GULF OF MEXICO. Marine Mammal Science, 14(July), 490–507.
Baird, R. W. (1998). An interaction between Pacific white-sided dolphins and a neonatal harbor porpoises. Mammalia, 62(1), 129–134.
Dehnhardt, G., Mauck, B., & Bleckmann, H. (1998). Seal whiskers detect water movements. Nature, 394(July), 235–236. https://doi.org/10.1038/28303
Dudzinski, K. M., & Newborough, D. (1997). Concurrent recording of dolphin behaviors, frequency-modulated tones, and pulsed vocalizations (including echolocation clicks) underwater with a swimmer-propelled system. In Proceedings of the Underwater BioSonar Systems and Bioacoustics Symposium. Underwater Acoustics Group.
Addink, M., Garcia Hartmann, M., & Couperus, B. (1997). A note on life-history parameters of the Atlantic white-sided dolphin (Lagenorhynchus acutus) from animals bycaught in the northeastern Atlantic. Report of the International Whaling Commission, (47), 637–639.
Kinze Carl, C., Addink, M., Smeenk, C., Garcia Hartmann, M., Richards, H. W., Sonntag Ralf, P., & Benke, H. (1997). The white-beaked dolphin (Lagenorhynchus albirostris) and the white-sided dolphin (Lagenorhynchus acutus) in the North and Baltic Seas: Review of available information. Report of the International Whaling Commission, (47), 675–681.
Kastelein, R. A., Dubbeldam, J. L., & Bakker, M. A. G. De. (1997). The anatomy of the walrus head (Odobenus rosmarus). Part 5 : The tongue and its function in walrus ecology. Aquatic Mammals, 23(1), 29–47.
Dudzinski, K. M., & Newborough, D. (1997). Concurrent recording of dolphin behaviors, frequency-modulated tones, and pulsed vocalizations (including echolocation clicks) underwater with a swimmer-propelled system. In Proceedings of the Underwater BioSonar Systems and Bioacoustics Symposium. Underwater Acoustics Group.
Miokovic, D., Kovacic, D., & Pribanic, S. (1997). Stomach content analysis of a bottlenose dolphin ({ITursiops truncatus}) from the Adriatic Sea. European Research on Cetaceans [Abstracts], 11(1), 149.
Moore, S. E., & Ridgway, S. H. (1996). Patterns of dolphin sound production and ovulation. Aquatic Mammals, 22(3), 175–184. Retrieved from http://www.aquaticmammalsjournal.org/share/AquaticMammalsIssueArchives/1996/AquaticMammals_22-03/22-03_Moore.pdf
Dudzinski, K. M. (1996). Communication and Behavior in the Atlantic Spotted Dolphins (Stenella frontalis): Relationships Between Vocal and Behavioral Activities. Society, (August), 218. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13398-014-0173-7.2
Cranford, T. W., Amundin, M., & Norris, K. S. (1996). Functional morphology and homology in the Odontoceti nasal complex implications for sound generation. Journal of Morphology, 228(3), 223–285. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-4687(199606)228:3<223::AID-JMOR1>3.0.CO;2-3
Dudzinski, K. M. (1996). Communication and Behavior in the Atlantic Spotted Dolphins (Stenella frontalis): Relationships Between Vocal and Behavioral Activities. Society, (August), 218. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13398-014-0173-7.2
Cranford, T. W., Amundin, M., & Norris, K. S. (1996). Functional morphology and homology in the odontocete nasal complex: Implications for sound generation. Journal of Morphology, 228(3), 223–285. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-4687(199606)228:3<223::AID-JMOR1>3.0.CO;2-3
Kastelein, R. A., Mosterd, P., Van Ligtenberg, C. L., & Verboom, W. C. (1996). Aerial hearing sensitivity tests with a male Pacific walrus (Odobenus rosmarus divergens), in the free field and with headphones. Aquatic Mammals, 22(2), 81–93.
Dudzinski, K. M., Frohoff, T. G., & Crane, N. L. (1995). Behavior of a lone female bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) with humans off the coast of Belize. Aquatic Mammals, 21(2), 149–153.
Clutton-Brock, T. H., & Parker, G. A. (1995). Punishment in animal societies. Nature, 373(6511), 209–216. https://doi.org/10.1038/373209a0
Kastelein, R. A., Nieuwstraten, S. H., & Verboom, W. C. (1995). Echolocation signals of harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) in light and complete darkness. Nachtigall, P.E., Lien, J., Au, W.W.L. & Read, A.J. [Eds]. Harbour Porpoises: Laboratory Studies to Reduce Bycatch. De Spil Publishers, Woerden, (xii), 1–168.
Kastelein Ed, R. A., Thomas Ed, J. A., & Nachtigall Ed, P. E. (1995). Sensory Systems of Aquatic Mammals. (R. A. Kastelein, J. A. Thomas, & P. E. Nachtigall, Eds.), Sensory Systems of Aquatic Mammals (Vol. 14). De Spil Publishers.
Dudzinski, K. M., Clark, C. W., & Wursig, B. (1995). A mobile video/acoustic system for simultaneously recording dolphin behavior and voacalizations underwate. Aquatic Mammals, 21(3), 187–193.
Dudzinski, K. M., Clark, C. W., & Wursig, B. (1995). A mobile video/acoustic system for simultaneously recording dolphin behavior and voacalizations underwate. Aquatic Mammals, 21(3), 187–193.
Dudzinski, K. M., Frohoff, T. G., & Crane, N. L. (1995). Behavior of a lone female bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) with humans off the coast of Belize. Aquatic Mammals.
Dudzinski, K. M., Frohoff, T. G., & Crane, N. L. (1995). Behavior of a lone female bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) with humans off the coast of Belize. Aquatic Mammals.
Kastelein, R. A., Nieuwstraten, S. H., & Verboom, W. C. (1995). Echolocation signals of harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) in light and complete darkness. Nachtigall, P.E., Lien, J., Au, W.W.L. & Read, A.J. [Eds]. Harbour Porpoises: Laboratory Studies to Reduce Bycatch. De Spil Publishers, Woerden, (xii), 1–168.
Dehnhardt, G. (1994). Tactile size discrimination by a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) using its mystacial vibrissae. Journal of Comparative Physiology A Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology, 175(6), 791–800. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7807420
Samuels, A., & Spraldin, T. (1994). QUANTITATIVE BEHAVIORAL STUDY OF BOTTLENOSE DOLPHINS IN SWIM-WITH-THE-DOLPHIN PROGRAMS IN THE UNITED STATES. Fisheries (Bethesda).
Gygax, L. (1993). Spatial movement patterns and behaviour of two captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): absence of stereotyped behaviour or lack of definition? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 38(3–4), 337–344. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(93)90031-J
Kastelein, R. A., & Lavaleije, M. S. S. (1992). Kastelein, R.A. and Lavaleije, M.S.S. (1992). Foreign bodies in the stomach of a female Harbour p.pdf. Aquatic Mammals.
Kastelein, R. A., & Lavaleije, M. S. S. (1992). Foreign bodies in the stomachs of female harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) from the North Sea. {A}quat {M}amm.
Amundin, M. (1991). Helium effects on the click frequency spectrum of the harbour porpoise, Phocoena phocoena. In Sound production in odontocetes with emphasis on the harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena), PhD Thesis (pp. 63–72). Univerity of Stockholm.
Quemard, A., Lacave, C., & Laneelle, G. (1991). Isoniazid inhibition of mycolic acid synthesis by cell extracts of sensitive and resistant strains of Mycobacterium aurum. Antimicrobial Agents and Chemotherapy, 35(6), 1035–1039.
Lacave, G. (1991). A survey of management practices for dolphin pregnancy – with two examples of birth complications. Aquatic Mammals, 17(1), 37–41.
Tyack, P. L., & Recchia, C. a. (1991). A datalogger to identify vocalizing dolphins. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 90(3), 1668–1671. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.401908
Quémard, a, Lacave, C., & Lanéelle, G. (1991). Isoniazid inhibition of mycolic acid synthesis by cell extracts of sensitive and resistant strains of Mycobacterium aurum. Antimicrobial Agents and Chemotherapy, 35(6), 1035–1039. Retrieved from http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=284282&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract
Von Fersen, L., & Güntürkün, O. (1990). Visual memory lateralization in pigeons. Neuropsychologia, 28(1), 1–7.
Kastelein, R. A., Stevens, S., & Mosterd, P. (1990). The tactile sensitivity of the mystacial vibrissae of a Pacific walrus. Part 2: Masking. Aquatic Mammals, 16(2), 78–87.
Ridgway, S. H. (1990). The central nervous system of the Bottlenose dolphin. In S. Leatherwood & R. R. Reeves (Eds.), The Bottlenose dolphin (pp. 69–97). Academic Press.
Packer, C., Scheel, D., & Pusey, A. E. (1990). Why Lions Form Groups: Food is Not Enough. The American Naturalist, 136, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1086/285079
Kastelein, R. A., & Gerrits, N. M. (1990). The anatomy of the Walrus head ( Odohenus rosmarus ). Part 1 : The skull. Aquatic Mammals, 16(3), 101–119.
Dudok Van Heel, W. H. and J. D. V. a N. D. E. R. T. (1988). A biological approach to dolphinarium water purification: II. A practical application: The Delfinaario in Tampere, Finland. Aquatic Mammals 14(3):92-106. 1988., 14, 15.
Reiss, D. (1988). Observations on the development of echolocation in young bottlenose dolphins. In P. E. Nachtigall & P. W. B. Moore (Eds.), Animal Sonar (pp. 121–127). Plenum Publishing Corp.
Daffé, M., Lacave, C., Lanéelle, M. a, & Lanéelle, G. (1987). Structure of the major triglycosyl phenol-phthiocerol of Mycobacterium tuberculosis (strain Canetti). European Journal of Biochemistry / FEBS, 167, 155–160. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3113946
Van Der Toorn, J. D. (1987). A biological approach to dolphinarium water purification: I. Theoretical aspects. Aquatic Mammals 13(3):83-92. 1987., 13(198 7), 10.
Cates, M. B., & Schroeder, J. P. (1986). The nutrition of acclimated vs newly captured Tursiops truncatus. Aquatic Mammals, 12(1), 17–20.
Amundin, M., & Andersen, S. H. (1983). Bony Nares Air Pressure and Nasal Plug Muscle Activity during Click Production in the Harbour Porpoise, Phocoena Phocoena, and the Bottlenosed Dolphin, Tursiops Truncatus. Journal of Experimental Biology, 105(1), 275–282. Retrieved from http://jeb.biologists.org/content/105/1/275.abstract%5Cnhttp://jeb.biologists.org/content/105/1/275.full.pdf
de Smet, W. M. A. (1977). The fate of old bottle-nosed dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, in nature as revealed by the condition of their skeleton. Aquatic Mammals, 5(3), 78–86.
Amundin, M., & Amundin, B. (1973). On the behaviour and study of the harbour porpoise, Phocoena phocoena, in the wild. Investigations on Cetacea, 5, 317–328. Retrieved from http://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=en&btnG=Search&q=intitle:On+the+behaviour+and+study+of+the+harbour+porpoise,+Phocoena+phocoena,+in+the+wild#0